Some thoughts on biolinguistics
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-6746.2015.2.22006Palabras clave:
Biolinguistics. Human language. Human mind. Human genome. Biology.Resumen
Naïve depictions of the biology of language are unable to treat the real complexity observed by biologists at all levels of analysis, and consequently they do not bring us closer to an accurate depiction of the nature of human language and the human mind. The aim of this essay is to show that if a real biolinguistics is intended to be achieved we ought to be compelled to go beyond these depictions.
Descargas
Citas
ANDERSON, S. R.; LIGHTFOOT, D. W. “The human language faculty as an organ”. Annu. Rev. Physiol., 62 (1999), p. 697-722. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.physiol.62.1.697
BALABAN, E. “Cognitive developmental biology: history, process and fortune’s wheel”. Cognition, 101 (2006), p. 298-332. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.006
BALARI, S.; BENÍTEZ-BURRACO, A.; LONGA V. M.; LORENZO G. “The fossils of language: What are they, who has them, how did they evolve?” In: C. BOECKX & K. K. GROHMANN (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Biolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2013, p. 489-523. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980435.028
BATES, E.; BRETHERTON, I.; SNYDER, L. From First Words to Grammar: Individual Differences and Dissociable Mechanisms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1988.
BENÍTEZ-BURRACO, A. Genes y lenguaje: aspectos ontogenéticos, filogenéticos y cognitivos [Genes and Language: Ontogenetic, Phylogenetic, and Cognitive Concerns]. Barcelona: Reverté, 2009.
BENÍTEZ-BURRACO, A. “Problematic aspects of the genetic analysis of the specific disorders of the language: FOXP2 as paradigm”. Neurologia, 27 (2012), p. 225-233. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nrleng.2012.05.005
BERWICK, R. C.; OKANOYA, K.; BECKERS, G. J. L.; BOLHUI, J. J. “Songs to syntax: the linguistics of birdsong”. Trends Cogn. Sci., 15 (2011), p. 115-121. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.002
BOECKX, C. “Biolinguistics: Forays into human cognitive biology”. J. Anthropol. Sci., 91 (2013). doi: 10.4436/JASS.91009.
BREUKER, C. J.; DEBAT, V.; KLINGENBERG, C. P. “Functional evo-devo”. Trends Ecol. Evol., 21 (2006), p. 488-492. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2006.06.003
CARROLL, S. B. Endless Forms Most Beautiful. The New Science of Evo Devo. New York: Norton & Company, 2005.
CÄSAR, C.; Byrne, R. W.; HOPPITT, W.; Young, R. J.; ZUBERBÜHLER, K. “Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls”. Anim. Behav., 84 (2013), p. 405-411. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.010
CHENEY, D. L.; SEYFARTH, R. M. How Monkeys See the World. Inside the Mind of Another Species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1990. DOI: https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226218526.001.0001
CHOMSKY, N. A. Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use. New York: Prager, 1986.
d’ERRICO, F.; HENSHILWOOD, C.; LAWSON, G.; VANHAEREN, M.; TILLIER, A.-M.; SORESSI, M.; BRESSON, F.; MAUREILLE, B.; NOWELL, A.; LAKARRA, J.; BACKWELL, L.; JULIEN, M. “Archeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music – An alternative multidisciplinary perspective”. J. World Prehist., 17 (2003), p. 1-70.
DEHAENE, S.; DUPOUX, E.; MEHLER, J.; COHEN, L.; PAULESU, E.; PERANI, D.; van de MOORTELE, P. F.; LEHÉRICY, S.; Le BIHAN, D. “Anatomical variability in the cortical representation of first and second language”. Neuroreport, 8 (1997), p. 3809-3815. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199712010-00030
ECO, U. A Theory of Semiotics (Advances in Semiotics). Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1976. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-15849-2
EMMOREY, K.; MCCULLOUGH, S. “The bimodal bilingual brain: effects of sign language experience”. Brain Lang, 109 (2009), p. 124-132. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.03.005
ENARD, W. “FOXP2 and the role of cortico-basal ganglia circuits in speech and language evolution”. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 21 (2011), p. 415-424. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.04.008
FALCARO, M.; PICKLES, A.; NEWBURY, D. F.; ADDIS, L.; BANFIELD, E.; FISHER, S. E.; MONACO, A. P.; SIMKIN, Z.; CONTI-RAMSDEN, G.; SLI Consortium. “Genetic and phenotypic effects of phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment”. Genes Brain Behav., 7 (2008), p. 393-402. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00364.x
FEDORENKO, E.; KANWISHER, N. “Neuroimaging of language: Why hasn’t a clearer picture emerged?” Lang. Linguist Compass, 3 (2009), p. 839-865. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00143.x
FEDORENKO, E.; BEHR, M. K.; KANWISHER, N. “Functional specificity for high-level linguistic processing in the human brain”. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 108 (2011), p. 16428-16433. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1112937108
FENSON, L.; BATES, E.; DALE, P.; GOODMAN, J.; REZNICK, J. S.; THAL, D. “Measuring variability in early child language: Don’t shoot the messenger”. Child Dev., 71 (2000), p. 323-328. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00147
FISHER, S. E.; SCHARFF, C. “FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language”. Trends Genet., 25 (2009), p. 166-177. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tig.2009.03.002
GIBSON, C. J.; GRUEN, J. R. “The human lexinome: genes of language and reading”. J. Commun. Disord., 41 (2008), p. 409-420. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.03.003
GRAHAM, S. A.; FISHER, S. E. “Decoding the genetics of speech and language”. Curr Opin. Neurobiol., 23 (2013), p. 43-51. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.006
GRAYBIEL, A. M. “Building action repertoires: memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia”. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol., 5 (1995), p. 733-741. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/0959-4388(95)80100-6
GRIFFITHS, P. E. “Evo-Devo meets the mind: towards a developmental evolutionary psychology”. In: BRANDON, R.; SANSOM, R. (Eds.). Integrating Evolution and Development: Form Theory to Practice. Cambridge: MIT Press, 2007, p. 195-225.
GRIFFITHS, P. E.; GRAY, R. D. “The developmental systems perspective: organismenvironment systems as units of evolution”. In: PRESTON, K.; PIGLIUCCI, M. (Eds.). Phenotypic Integration: Studying the Ecology and Evolution of Complex Phenotypes. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2004, p. 409-431.
GRODZINSKY, Y. “The picture of the linguistic brain: how sharp can it be? Reply to Fedorenko & Kanwisher”. Lang. Linguist Compass, 4 (2010), p. 605-622. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00222.x
GUSELLA, J. F.; MACDONALD, M. E. “Huntington’s disease: seeing the pathogenic process through a genetic lens”. Trends Biochem. Sci., 31 (2006), p. 533-540. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tibs.2006.06.009
HANCOCK, R.; BEVER, T. G. “Genetic factors and normal variation in the organization of language”. Biolinguistics, 7 (2013), p. 75-95. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5964/bioling.8971
HOLLOWAY, R. L. “Human paleontological evidence relevant to language behaviour”. Hum. Neurobiol., 2 (1983), p. 105-114.
KARMILOFF-SMITH, A. “Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disorders”. Dev. Psychol., 45 (2009), p. 56-63. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014506
KARMILOFF-SMITH, A. “A developmental perspective on modularity”. In: GLATZEDER, B. M.; GOEL, V.; von MÜLLER, A. (Eds.). Towards a Theory of Thinking. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, 2010, p. 179-187. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-03129-8_12
KITANO, H. “Biological robustness”. Nature Rev. Genet., 5 (2004), p. 826-837. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg1471
KRAUSE, J.; LALUEZA-FOX, C.; ORLANDO, L.; ENARD, W.; GREEN, R. E.;, BURBANO, H. A.; HUBLIN, J. J.; HÄNNI, C.; FORTEA, J.; de la RASILLA, M.; BERTRANPETIT, J.; ROSAS, A.; PÄÄBO, S. “The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals”. Curr. Biol., 17 (2007), p. 1908-1912. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008
KURT, S.; FISHER, S. E.; EHRET, G. “Foxp2 mutations impair auditory motor association learning”. PLoS One, 7 (2012), e33130. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033130
LAI, C. S.; FISHER, S. E.; HURST, J. A.; VARGHA-KHADEM, F.; MONACO, A. P. “A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder”. Nature, 413 (2001), p. 519-523. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/35097076
LENNEBERG, E. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley, 1967. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/21548331.1967.11707799
LIGHTFOOT, D. The Development of Language. Acquisition, Change, and Evolution. Oxford, Malden: Blackwell, 1999.
LINEBARGER, M. C. “Agrammatism as evidence about grammar”. Brain Lang, 50 (1995), p. 52-91. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1995.1040
LOVE, A. C. “Functional homology and homology of function: Biological concepts and philosophical consequences”. Biol. Philos., 22 (2007), p. 691-708. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-007-9093-7
MARTÍNEZ, I.; ROSA, M.; ARSUAGA, J. L.; JARABO, P.; QUAM, R.; LORENZO, C.; GRACIA, A.; CARRETERO, J. M.; BERMÚDEZ de CASTRO, J. M.; CARBONELL, E. “Auditory capacities in Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sierra de Atapuerca in Spain”. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 101 (2004), p. 9976-9981. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0403595101
MELLARS, P. The impossible coincidence. A single-species model for the origins of modern human behavior. Evol. Anthropol., 14 (2005), p. 12-27. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20037
MITHEN, S. The Singing Neanderthals. The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2006.
MÜLLER, G. B.; NEWMAN, S. A. (Eds.). “Evolutionary innovation and morphological novelty”. J. Exp. Zool. B Mol. Dev. Evol., 304 (2005), p. 485-486. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/jez.b.21080
OYAMA, S.; GRIFFITHS, P. E.; GRAY, R. D. (Eds.). Cycles of Contingencies: Developmental Systems and Evolution. [s.l.]: MIT Press, 2001.
PADOVANI, A.; COSSEDDU, M.; PREMI, E.; ARCHETTI, S.; PAPETTI, A.; AGOSTI, C.; BIGNI, B.; CERINI, C.; PAGHERA, B.; BELLELLI, G.; BORRONI, B. “The speech and language FOXP2 gene modulates the phenotype of frontotemporal lobar degeneration”. J. Alzheimers Dis., 22 (2010), p. 923-931. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2010-101206
PIATELLI-PALMARINI, M.; URIAGEREKA, J. “A geneticist’s dream, a linguist’s nightmare: The case of FOXP2”. In: Di SCIULLO, A. M.; BOECKX, C. (Eds.). The Biolinguistic Enterprise. New Perspectives on the Evolution and Nature of the Human Language Faculty. New York: Oxford University Press, 2011, p. 100-125.
PINKER, S.; BLOOM, P. “Natural language and natural selection”. Behav. Brain Sci., 13 (1990), p. 707-784. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00081061
POEPPEL, D. “The maps problem and the mapping problem: two challenges for a cognitive neuroscience of speech and language”. Cogn. Neuropsychol., 29 (2012), p. 34-55. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2012.710600
POEPPEL, D.; EMBICK, D. “Defining the relation between linguistics and neuroscience”. In: CUTLER, A. (Ed.). Twenty-first Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2005, p. 103-120.
PRAT, C. S.; JUST, M. A. “Exploring the neural dynamics underpinning individual differences in sentence comprehension”. Cereb. Cortex, 21 (2011), p. 1747-1760. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq241
RAMUS, F.; ROSEN, S.; DAKIN, S. C.; DAY, B. L.; CASTELLOTE, J. M.; WHITE, S.; FRITH, U. “Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults”. Brain, 126 (2003), p. 841-865. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg076
SHAYWITZ, S. E.; SHAYWITZ, B. A.; PUGH, K. R.; FULBRIGHT, R. K.; CONSTABLE, R. T.; MENCL, W. E.; SHANKWEILER, D. P.; LIBERMAN, A. M.; SKUDLARSKI, P.; FLETCHER, J. M.; KATZ, L.; MARCHIONE, K. E.; LACADIE, C.; GATENBY, C.; GORE, J. C. “Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia”. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 95 (1998), p. 2636-2641. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.5.2636
SHU, W.; YANG, H.; ZHANG, L.; LU, M. M.; MORRISEY, E. E. “Characterization of a new subfamily of wingedhelix/forkhead (Fox) genes that are expressed in the lung and act as transcriptional repressors”. J. Biol. Chem., 276 (2001), p. 27488- 27497. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M100636200
SILVERSTEIN, M. “Shifters, linguistic categories, and cultural description”. In: BASSO, K. H.; SELBY, H. A. (Eds.). Meaning in Anthropology. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press, 1976, p. 11-55.
SIROIS, S.; SPRATLING, M.; THOMAS, M. S.; WESTERMANN, G.; MARESCHAL, D.; JOHNSON, M. H. “Précis of neuroconstructivism: how the brain constructs cognition”. Behav. Brain Sci., 31 (2008), p. 321-331. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0800407X
SMITH, N. V.; TSIMPLI, I. M. The Mind of a Savant: Language-Learning and Modularity. Oxford: Blackwell, 1995.
SORGER, B.; GOEBEL, R.; SCHILTZ, C.; ROSSION, B. “Understanding the functional neuroanatomy of acquired prosopagnosia”. Neuroimage, 35 (2007), p. 836-852. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.051
STATE, M. W. “The erosion of phenotypic specificity in psychiatric genetics: emerging lessons from CNTNAP2”. Biol. Psychiatry, 69 (2011), p. 816-817. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.021
TATTERSALL, I. Becoming Human: Evolution and Human Uniqueness. New York: Harcourt Brace, 1998.
THOMAS, M. S.; ANNAZ, D.; ANSARI, D.; SCERIF, G.; JARROLD, C.; KARMILOFFSMITH, A. “Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders”. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res., 52 (2009), p. 336-358. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0144)
VARGHA-KHADEM, F.; WATKINS, K. E.; ALCOCK, K. J.; FLETCHER, P.; PASSINGHAM, R. E. “Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder”. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 92 (1995), p. 930-933. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.92.3.930
WAGNER, G. P. “Homologues, natural kinds and the evolution of modularity”. Amer. Zool., 36 (1996), p. 36-43. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/36.1.36
WAGNER, G. P.; ALTENBERG, L. “Complex adaptations and the evolution of evolvability”. Evolution, 50 (1996), p. 967-976. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1996.tb02339.x
WATKINS, K. E., DRONKERS, N. F. and VARGHA-KHADEM, F. “Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia”. Brain, 125 (2002), p. 452-464. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf058
WEST-EBERHARD, M. J. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Nueva York: Oxford University Press, 2003. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195122343.003.0008
WEST-EBERHARD, M. J. “Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences”. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 102 (2005), p. 6543-6549. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0501844102
WEXLER, K. “Lenneberg’s dream: Learning, normal language development, and Specific Language Impairment”. In: Levy , Y.; Schaeffer, J. (Eds.). Language Competence across Populations. Toward a Definition of Specific Language Impairment. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2003, p. 11-61.
WINTHER, R. G. “Varieties of modules: kinds, levels, origins, and behaviors”. J. Exp. Zool., 291 (2001), p. 116-129. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/jez.1064
WYNN, T.; COOLIDGE, F. L. “The expert Neandertal mind”. J. Hum. Evol., 46 (2004), p. 467-487. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.01.005
ZILLES, K.; AMUNTS, K. “Receptor mapping: architecture of the human cerebral cortex”. Curr. Opin. Neurol., 22 (2009), p. 331-339. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0b013e32832d95db
Descargas
Publicado
Cómo citar
Número
Sección
Licencia
Derechos de Autor
La sumisión de originales para la Revista Veritas implica la transferencia, por los autores, de los derechos de publicación. El copyright de los artículos de esta revista es el autor, junto con los derechos de la revista a la primera publicación. Los autores sólo podrán utilizar los mismos resultados en otras publicaciones indicando claramente a Revista Veritas como el medio de la publicación original.
Creative Commons License
Excepto donde especificado de modo diferente, se aplican a la materia publicada en este periódico los términos de una licencia Creative Commons Atribución 4.0 Internacional, que permite el uso irrestricto, la distribución y la reproducción en cualquier medio siempre y cuando la publicación original sea correctamente citada. Copyright: © 2006-2020 EDIPUCRS