El ruido visual dinámico afecta la tarea de imagen mental basada en el paradigma retro-cue

Autores/as

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2024.1.42132

Palabras clave:

memoria de trabajo, atención, imagen mental, conciencia

Resumen

El efecto retro-cue (RCE) es conocido en las tareas de memoria por producir un aumento en la precisión y una disminución en el tiempo de respuesta. La retro-señal trae la información al foco de atención en un estado activo, pero no se sabe si se recupera como una imagen mental consciente. En este estudio, investigamos si la retro-señal lleva al elemento señalado a un estado activo, permitiendo su conciencia fenomenológica. En cada ensayo, los participantes calificaron la intensidad del elemento retro-indicado y luego lo reconocieron. Se utilizó la interferencia visual perceptiva para certificar la naturaleza visual de los elementos recuperados. Nuestros resultados mostraron un RCE significativo en el reconocimiento de la memoria, pero no en el índice de viveza. La viveza de la imagen mental y las tareas de reconocimiento se basan en diferentes procesos cognitivos y están sujetas a diferentes interferencias, pero a pesar de los diferentes procesos cognitivos, existe una correlación entre ellos.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.

Biografía del autor/a

Mariana Ribeiro Maniglia, Universidad de Ribeirão Preto (UNAERP), Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brasil.

Doctor en Psicobiología por la Universidad de São Paulo (USP), en Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brasil. Psicóloga y profesora de la carrera de Psicología de la Universidad de Ribeirão Preto (UNAERP), en Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brasil.

César Alexis Galera, Universidad de São Paulo (USP), Facultad de Filosofía, Ciencias y Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Ribeirão Preto, SP Brasil.

Doctor en Psicología por la Universidad de São Paulo (USP), en São Paulo, SP, Brasil; con un posdoctorado de la Universidad Concordia, en Montreal, Canadá. Profesor Titular de Psicología de la Facultad de Filosofía, Ciencias y Letras de Ribeirão Preto – Universidad de São Paulo (USP), en Ribeirão Preto, SP, Brasil.

Citas

Albers, A. M., Kok, P., Toni, I., Dijkerman, H. C., & de Lange, F. P. (2013). Shared Representations for Working Memory and Mental Imagery in Early Visual Cortex. Current Biology, 23(15), 1427-1431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.065 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.065

Amedi, A., Malach, R., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Negative BOLD Differentiates Visual Imagery and Perception. Neuron, 48(5), 859-872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.032 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.032

Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. (pp. xviii, 412). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528012.001.0001 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198528012.001.0001

Baddeley, A. D., & Andrade, J. (2000). Working memory and the vividness of imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 126-145. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.1.126 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.129.1.126

Bona, S., Cattaneo, Z., Vecchi, T., Soto, D., & Silvanto, J. (2013). Metacognition of visual short-term memory: Dissociation between objective and subjective components of VSTM. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00062 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00062

Bona, S., & Silvanto, J. (2014). Accuracy and Confidence of Visual Short-Term Memory Do Not Go Hand-In-Hand: Behavioral and Neural Dissociations. PLOS ONE, 9(3), e90808. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090808 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090808

Cui, X., Jeter, C., Yang, D., Montague, P., & Eagleman, D. (2007). Vividness of mental imagery: Individual variability can be measured objectively. Vision Research, 47(4), 474-478. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.11.013

D’Angiulli, A., & Reeves, A. (2002). Generating visual mental images: Latency and vividness are inversely related. Memory & Cognition, 30(8), 1179-1188. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213401 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213401

Dijkstra, N., Bosch, S. E., & van Gerven, M. A. (2019). Shared neural mechanisms of visual perception and imagery. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(5), 423-434. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.02.004

Dijkstra, N., Bosch, S. E., & van Gerven, M. A. J. (2017). Vividness of visual imagery depends on the neural overlap with perception in visual areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(5), 1367-1373. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3022-16.2016 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3022-16.2016

El Haj, M., Gallouj, K., & Antoine, P. (2019). Mental imagery and autobiographical memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 33(5), 609. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000521

Gayet, S., Guggenmos, M., Christophel, T. B., Haynes, J.-D., Paffen, C. L., Van der Stigchel, S., & Sterzer, P. (2017). Visual working memory enhances the neural response to matching visual input. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(28), 6638-6647. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3418-16.2017

Griffin, I. C., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). Orienting Attention to Locations in Internal Representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(8), 1176-1194. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903322598139 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903322598139

Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., Engell, A. D., & McCulloch, K. C. (2009). Implicit working memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 665-678. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.04.003 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.04.003

Huang Y, Pollick F, Liu M, Zhang D. (2021) Shared and Independent Neural Representation Between Visual Perception and Mental Imagery. Research Square. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-788978/v1 DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-788978/v1

Jacob, J., Jacobs, C., & Silvanto, J. (2015). Attention, working memory, and phenomenal experience of WM content: Memory levels determined by different types of top-down modulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1603-1603. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01603 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01603

Jacobs, C., Schwarzkopf, D. S., & Silvanto, J. (2018). Visual working memory performance in aphantasia. Cortex, 105, 61-73. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.014

Jacobs, C., & Silvanto, J. (2015). How is working memory content consciously experienced? The ‘conscious copy’ model of WM introspection. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 55, 510-519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.003 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.003

Keogh, R., & Pearson, J. (2014). The sensory strength of voluntary visual imagery predicts visual working memory capacity. Journal of Vision, 14(12), 7-7. https://doi.org/10.1167/14.12.7 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1167/14.12.7

Keogh, R., & Pearson, J. (2018). The blind mind: No sensory visual imagery in aphantasia. The Eye’s Mind - Visual Imagination, Neuroscience and the Humanities, 105, 53-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.012 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.012

Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/35090055

Kreiman, G., Koch, C., & Fried, I. (2000). Imagery neurons in the human brain. Nature, 408, 357. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/35042575

Lepsien, J., & Nobre, A. C. (2006). Cognitive control of attention in the human brain: Insights from orienting attention to mental representations. Control of Attention and Actions, 1105(1), 20-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.033 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.033

Li, Z., Tong, M., Chen, S., & Qian, J. (2021). Effect of attentional selection on working memory for depth in a retro-cueing paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 49(4), 747-757. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-020-01123-4

Makovski, T., & Jiang, Y. (2008). Distributing versus focusing attention in visual short-term memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 1072-1078. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193093 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193093

Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British Journal of Psychology, 64(1), 17-24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x

McConnell, J., & Quinn, J. G. (2000). Interference in Visual Working Memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 53(1), 53-67. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755873 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/713755873

Nanay, B. (2018). Multimodal mental imagery. Cortex, 105, 125-134. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.006

Nanay, B. (2021). Unconscious mental imagery. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1817), 20190689. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0689

Niklaus, M., Singmann, H., & Oberauer, K. (2019). Two distinct mechanisms of selection in working memory: Additive last-item and retro-cue benefits. Cognition, 183, 282-302. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.11.015

Pearson, D. G. (2001). Imagery and the visuo-spatial sketchpad. In Working memory in perspective (pp. 33-59). Psychology Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203194157_chapter_2

Pearson, J. (2019). The human imagination: The cognitive neuroscience of visual mental imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(10), 624–634. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0202-9

Pearson, J., Rademaker, R. L., & Tong, F. (2011). Evaluating the Mind’s Eye: The Metacognition of Visual Imagery. Psychological Science, 22(12), 1535-1542. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417134 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417134

Pounder, Z., Jacob, J., Evans, S., Loveday, C., Eardley, A., & Silvanto, J. (2021). Individuals with congenital aphantasia show no significant neuropsychological deficits on imagery-related memory tasks. PsyArXiv, 1-22. DOI: https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/gqayt

Quinn, J. G., & McConnell, J. (2006). The interval for interference in conscious visual imagery. Memory, 14(2), 241-252. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210500210019 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210500210019

Runge, M. S., Cheung, M. W., & D’Angiulli, A. (2017). Meta-analytic comparison of trial-versus questionnaire-based vividness reportability across behavioral, cognitive and neural measurements of imagery. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2017(1), nix006. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/nix006

Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime: A user’s guide. Psychology Software Tools.

Sheldon, S., Amaral, R., & Levine, B. (2017). Individual differences in visual imagery determine how event information is remembered. Memory, 25(3), 360-369. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1178777 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1178777

Snodgrass, J. G., & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(1), 34-50. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.34 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.117.1.34

Soto, D., Mäntylä, T., & Silvanto, J. (2011). Working memory without consciousness. Current Biology, 21(22), R912–R913. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.049 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.049

Souza, A. S., & Oberauer, K. (2016). In search of the focus of attention in working memory: 13 years of the retro-cue effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(7), 1839-1860. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1108-5 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1108-5

Souza, A. S., Rerko, L., & Oberauer, K. (2016). Getting more from visual working memory: Retro-cues enhance retrieval and protect from visual interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(6), 890. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000192

Thompson, E. (2007). Look again: Phenomenology and mental imagery. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(1–2). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-006-9031-1

Valenti, L., & Galera, C. (2020). Dynamic visual noise has the same effect on visual memory and visual imagery tasks. Psychology & Neuroscience, 13(1), 114-125. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000183 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000183

van der Meulen, M., Logie, R. H., & Sala, S. D. (2009). Selective interference with image retention and generation: Evidence for the workspace model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1568-1580. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802483800 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802483800

Wilson, A. C., Schwannauer, M., McLaughlin, A., Ashworth, F., & Chan, S. W. (2018). Vividness of positive mental imagery predicts positive emotional response to visually presented Project Soothe pictures. British Journal of Psychology, 109(2), 259-276. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12267

Zerr, P., Gayet, S., van den Esschert, F., Kappen, M., Olah, Z., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2021). The development of retro-cue benefits with extensive practice: Implications for capacity estimation and attentional states in visual working memory. Memory & Cognition, 1-14. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-021-01138-5

Descargas

Publicado

2024-10-21

Cómo citar

Ribeiro Maniglia, M., & Alexis Galera, C. (2024). El ruido visual dinámico afecta la tarea de imagen mental basada en el paradigma retro-cue. Psico, 55(1), e42132. https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2024.1.42132

Artículos similares

<< < 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 > >> 

También puede Iniciar una búsqueda de similitud avanzada para este artículo.